7 Chapter 7: Emotional Life and Relationships
Gowri Parameswaran
Learning Goals
- Learn about the development of emotions and how they relate to self-regulation
- Explore the development of friendships and intimate connections
- Understand the role of romantic growth in youth life
- Know Social Reproduction Theory (SRT)
- Explore some major issues surrounding youth relationships
Chapter Outline
- Introduction
- Emotional Self-Regulation and Relationships
- o Friendships
- o Intimacy
- o Dating
- o Violence in Dating Relationships
- o Romantic Relationships
- Sociological Frameworks Exploring Relationships
- o Social Reproduction Theory
- o School Status and Maintenance
- Fostering Interethnic Relationships
- Important Issues Related to Youth Relationships
- o Cyber-bullying
- o Sexuality Education
- o Coping with Loss and Grieving
- Conclusion
- Glossary of Terms
- References
Introduction
The chapter on the physical development of adolescents enumerated on the dramatic biological changes in youth as their bodies change from prepubescent to being fully developed and sexually mature. Their brains similarly undergo rapid transformation with new connections being formed and unused ones being pruned. All these changes have deep implications for their emotional growth during adolescence. Popular culture depicts adolescent emotions as being unstable and quick changing. As discussed in earlier chapters, this is only part of the truth about youth life and is a result of not just biological changes, but the social, political and economic contexts that adolescents live and function in. In this chapter, we will examine some of the changes that adolescents go through and the impact on their lives and relationships.
Emotional Self-Regulation and Relationships
Children and youth demonstrate an increasing capacity to control their emotion by exercising a wide range of strategies that are increasingly effective through adolescence, though the development of emotional control extend well into adulthood. The strategies might include distracting oneself from thinking about stressful situations, taking on challenging tasks, and engaging in planful action to eliminate the stress associated with impulsive behavior (Gardner, Dishion & Connell, 2008; Gestsdóttir & Lerner, 2007; Zimmerman, 2000). One of the most famous techniques used to explore young children’s self-regulation strategies and capacity has been termed the marshmallow experiment, originally conducted by Walter Mischel in 1972 (Mischel, Yoda and Rodriguez, 1972; University of Rochester, 2012). The experiment involved having children get a reward immediately but increasing the amount of the reward if they waited until the experimenter got back to the laboratory after an interruption. The reward consisted of a marshmallow. Mischel found that there were individual differences in how long and if the young participants waited for the experiment to come back to double their reward. When the participants were followed for several years after, the children who waited for a bigger reward were more successful than the children who did not wait. Replications of the experiment however did not quite substantiate this finding. The experiment was also biased towards upper and middle-class children. The authors of the study assert that the reliability of getting rewards is intimately linked with self-regulation-oriented behaviors (Calarco, 2001). In one recent German and Cameroonian children were tested using the Marshmallow experiment. Cameroonian children waited longer for their reward, and a lager percentage of Cameroonian children were able to resist the temptation to eat the sweet on the table right away. Interestingly, the Cameroonian participants did not need to distract themselves by self-talk and other methods to resist the temptation to eat the reward.
Even during their earliest months, infants take steps to soothe themselves in stressful situations by turning away from the source of an unwanted stimulation and engaging in other self-soothing techniques like sucking (Rothbart, Ziaie & O’Boyle, 1992). When parents respond to infants’ emotional cues, babies are less likely to be stressed and fussy. During the second year of life, capacity to express themselves through language allows for additional control of emotions. They can now talk to themselves or transform their goals to reduce stress. Parents and other adults can model self-talk that is positive and self-affirming for young people thereby offering them paths to soothe themselves (Diener & Mangelsdorf, 1999; Garner, 1995).
In adolescence, children become increasingly capable of recognizing different emotions in others (Thomas, Debellis, Graham & LaBar, 2007) and in themselves (Johnson et. al, 2002). At the same time, they learn appropriate ways to express the complex emotions that they experience (Collier & Collier, 2014). Youth experience fears and anxieties related to their relationships with significant others and their achievement on tasks though there are cultural differences in how these fears are experienced and expressed (George & Odger, 2015). Some of the more recent fears among youth are related to their engagement with digital technology, and this is often reflected in their offline behavior as well. Adolescents’ digital interactions are evidenced to be related to sleep disturbances and fears of being bullied or trolled, which in turn have consequences for their feelings of anxieties. In order to control the feelings of insecurity and reduce stress, youth engage in both active problem-solving strategies and more internally focused emotional control (Riediger & Klipker, 2014; Rood et. al., 2012). Children who feel as if they do have better control over their emotions are more likely to experience optimism and hopefulness (Shapiro, 1997; Siu & Shek, 2010).
At the same time, all through middle school and high school, children become increasingly knowledgeable about the rules around emotional expression (Lutz, 1996; Stearns & Stearns, 1989). They learn that negative emotions do not evoke friendly expression from other people while happy positive emotions do (Stearns & Stearns, 1989). This feedback loop allows youth to regulate their emotions in the presence of other people. Adolescents learn how to use increasingly differentiated language to express their emotional states and seek assistance in dealing with situations where they feel powerless (Greenberg, 2006). At the same time, the connection between language and emotional regulation becomes increasingly divergent for boys and girls as they go through adolescence. While girls are taught to value emotional expression and their own connections to other people in their lives, boys are taught very early that the free expression of pro-nurturing feelings need to be held back for them to be valued as boys and men (Lewis & Simpson, 2007).
Children and youth learn empathy and understanding of other people’s emotions over the period of their development (Hoffman, 1991). The roots of these capabilities lie in early childhood and perhaps even in infancy when very young children use social referencing extensively; the term refers to acquiring emotional cues from their caregivers’ facial expressions in order to determine appropriate responses in unfamiliar circumstances (Walden & Ogan, 1988). The need to feel safe drives young children to carefully read any situation through their caregivers’ emotional expressions (Denham, 1998). During their early childhood, children are increasingly adept at identifying other people’s emotions accurately and can ascribe reasons and predict the consequences of different emotions on future behavior (Eggum et al., 2011). In later childhood, studies demonstrate that participants can understand that people can express emotions they do not feel and that they can think one thing and say another (Harris, 1994). When caregivers use a large vocabulary to express their emotional states, children obtain a deeper understanding of affective states – this translates into better problem-solving skills, empathy, emotionally mature behavior and being perceived as prosocial by others around the child (Cutting & Dunn, 1999). Thus, empathy and the capacity to read other people’s emotions has roots in individual predisposition, early experiences and cultural values, but experts agree that it is a precursor to altruistic and prosocial behavior. Regulating one’s own emotion is important in order to empathize with other people’s needs while hostile, impulsive and aggressive behaviors prevent one from taking in other perspectives (Hoffman, 2001). There is a strong correlation between self-regulation, empathy and emotional intelligence (Kit Welchlin, 2014).
Friendships
Youth networks consist of cliques which are small groups of people closely connected to each other which are in turn loosely connected to other small cliques in larger networks called crowds (Hartup, 1993). Though elementary school children spend much of their social lives in cliques, they move on to larger groups as they get older. Children do maintain their closest ties with a small group of friends. Within these small networks, youth create their own frameworks of meaning. Selective and often creative use of language and ritualized behavior form important elements to both the meaning-making aspects of their lives in the social communities and to cementing their bonds with each other (Brown & Klute, 2003).
Friendships from the earliest years in school are marked by seeking similarities in terms of race and sex. As they enter adolescence, some youth begin to cross gender boundaries and seek friendships across genders. Well-adjusted boys and girls demonstrate a marked capacity to form friendships with both same sex and other sex teens. Girls typically value closeness and openness in their friendships while boys are more likely to engage in activities together. In the context of such gendered friendship styles, boys learn trust and intimacy when they have close friends who are girls (CSU Fullerton HCOM, 2013). Race, class, gender and neighborhood characteristics all play key roles in determining who children and youth befriend, how the friendship unfolds, the spaces they occupy in a person’s life, the relationship these peer connections have to family life and the impact they have on the person’s life (Clark & Ayers, 1992; Graham, Cohen, Zbikowski & Secrist, 1998).
From very early in childhood when children are given a choice, they select friends who are most like them temperamentally; thus, aggressive children seem to seek out other aggressive children to associate with (Gleason, Gower, Hohmann & Gleason, 2005). Researchers have found that even when children exhibit behavioral problems, they find similar levels of support and loyalty from their friends as young people without behavioral issues. This is one reason that there is an intensifying of conduct issues over the course of childhood as youth reward behaviors that they approve of in each other (Gifford-Smith, Dodge, Dishion & McCord, 2005; Seidman et al (1999). Adolescents often find themselves very vulnerable in terms of their social relationships and young people with conduct issues find a great deal of support from each other as they find themselves increasingly isolated from others in their peer group.
There has been much research conducted on the cross-cultural variations in friendship patterns. North American adolescents tend to spend less time on homework and with family than they do with peers as compared to Asian youth (Larsen & Verma, 1999); however, there are intra-national differences in this pattern of findings. African American youth report feeling closer to their parents and spending more time with family members than their white peers (Bengtson, 2001). Similarly, youth from middle class homes typically tended to be more instrumental in their friendship patterns, forming and breaking connections depending on changes in their interests and their activities. Students from working class homes lacking these extracurricular outlets have more long-lasting relationships often with youth from their neighborhoods and within their extended family (Boiché, Sarrazin & Chanal, 2015). Young people from homes with unstable living conditions must relocate frequently leaving them with little time to form lasting friendships. In addition, living in unsafe environments forces children to have a cautious approach to people and unable to trust in others (Anderson & Leventhal, 2017; Faris & Felmlee, 2018).
The quality and the type of friendships they have and the peer networks they maintain have a significant impact on the lives of children and adolescents. Research has pointed to the impact of peer networks in fostering delinquency and aggression. Earlier research posited that isolation led young people to antisocial behavior (Rubin & Mills, 1988); today there is substantial evidence that points to the notion that friendship qualities do not differ for conforming and nonconforming adolescents but teens with different temperaments seek others who share their values and framework towards school and society (Rulison, Gest & Osgood, 2015; Young et al., 2014). Similarly, it has been found that suicide ideation and body dysmorphia can be fostered by peer groups (Fortuin, Van Geel & Vedder, 2015; Kenny et al., 2017). Thus, self-destructive behavior can be disrupted by peers as well. Peers tend to reward others who exhibit behaviors that conform to their own cherished values. In a subset of studies, there has been racial differences in peer influence on behavior; Black teens tend to be less susceptible to peer pressure than white teens. This correlates with the strong attachment that Black teens exhibit towards their families (Giordano, Cernkovich & DeMaris, 1993).
Intimacy
Harry Stack Sullivan (1937) offered one of the most comprehensive theories describing the development of intimacy between people before and during adolescence. According to Sullivan, our capacity for intimacy predates adolescence and the quality of our earliest intimate relationships with others, including friends and family predict our capacity for close relationships during adolescence. He believed that it is healthy for adolescents to experiment with different roles in their relationship as they make novel attempts to experience intimacy and sexual fulfillment within the same relationship. Some youth may not attempt a sexual relationship but may have close intimate relationships with same sex and other sex peers while others may engage in sex without seeking intimacy from the same person. By the time they reach late adolescents, a mature young person is ready to address intimacy needs in a healthy fashion (Buhrmester & Furman, 1987).
While early adolescents describe their relationships as companionships within which they engage in activities together, later in youth, individuals increasingly describe their friendships as including loyalty and intimacy. For girls, these qualities are especially important in their relationships and any perceived betrayal of the relationship is accompanied by emotional upheaval. As they get older, teens learn to deal with conflict with their best friends through negotiation and compromise rather than dominance and insisting on getting their way. During the same period, adolescents become increasingly knowledgeable about their closest friends’ deepest secrets and know things about them that others do not. Over the course of adolescence, they are increasingly likely to show more care and concern for their friends and predict their closest friends’ responses to situations better than casual acquaintances (Hey, 1997).
Research with white teens has demonstrated that girls define their relationships to close friends by highlighting the self-disclosure, intimacy and care that they share. Boys tend to be less exclusive and more controlling in their friendships (Shulman et al., 1997). While the heightened empathy and understanding among girls’ close relationships is comforting, it can also lead to excessive rumination leading to increased depression and anxiety for the participants. Similarly, the closeness among girls makes conflict more emotionally painful. Boys’ conflicts often are a result of exertion of power and control and gets resolved on its own without the trauma that accompanies girls’ conflicts (Goodwin, 2006). Towards the end of adolescence there are few gender differences in the quality of close relationships that boys and girls share (Davis, 2012; Valkenberg, Sumter & Peter, 2011). The pattern of gender differences in self-disclosure is varied for youth from other backgrounds with Black teens exhibiting few gender differences in the quality of close friendships through adolescence (Miller & Benson, 1999).
While adolescents’ intimacy with their peers and romantic partners grow, so does their intimacy with their parents especially later in adolescence and into adulthood. Studies have found that the quality of relationships that youth have with their peers and romantic partners is related to the quality of the relationships they enjoy at home. In some ways, teens find it easier to express negative emotions to their parents since they are convinced that the ties are less likely to break (Larson & Asmussen, 1991). While there are ethnic group differences in the relationship between youth and their parents, actual behavior tends to be very similar across cultural groups with most adolescents naming their parents as being very significant in terms of sources of support (Helson, Vollebergh & Meese, 2000). In the modern social context in the west, relationships with siblings and the extended family tends to diminish in importance through adolescence; Black youth have a deep relationship with their grandparents when they have an unmarried or partnered mother (Stoneman & Brody, 1993; Wilson, 1986). Often, youth tend to seek help and support from different sources in different areas as they navigate adolescence. For many teens, there is no one to offer a helping hand when needed, especially when the adolescent belongs to a more vulnerable group in terms of race, social class, sexual and gender orientation or a discriminated religious group.
Dating
Over the last few decades, the age of marriage first declined and then increased for both men and women ((Rotz, 2016). New research shows that boys have the same needs for emotional intimacy that girls have but find themselves inadequately prepared by society to engage with cross-sex intimacy in an authentic fashion (Coontz, 2015). The age at which adolescents date is influenced by several factors – age of sexual maturation is one; the earlier a teen matures the earlier they begin to date. The attitude that communities hold around issues of dating and the behavior of teens’ peer groups, influence the age at which youth begin dating (Besharov & Gardier, 1993).
Dating could involve anything from casual outings in mixed sex groups to having an intimate relationship with one person. Usually, the former precedes the latter and the transition to serious dating is hard for most teens because of their limited experience and awkwardness around initiating and maintaining romantic relationships. Adolescent relationships tend to involve more conflict around jealousy and betrayal as compared to young adult romantic connections. By late adolescence, issues regarding commitment and intimacy begin to enter romantic partnerships even though relationships are still unstable (Connolly & McIsaac, 2011; Connolly et al., 2004). For gay, lesbian, bisexual and transgender individuals, the development of their intimate relationships may have a different trajectory because of the discrimination and the stigma attached to their choices. Despite the enormous progress being made in the area, LGBTQ+ youth face censure when they attempt to express intimacy in public; same sex friends may misinterpret their actions and they may have to hide their desires in order to be accepted (Furman, Brown & Feiring, 1999).
Violence in Dating Relationships
Violence and aggression are all too common among youth in both heterosexual and homosexual relationships (Amanda Maver, 2014). It can include physical, sexual, emotional and psychologically aggressive behavior with romantic partners. Nearly 23 % of girls and 14 % of boys experience violence from their dating partner before the age of 18. Unlike intimate partner abuse in adulthood, adolescents of both genders are almost equally likely to be perpetrators of the violence because, physically, boys and girls are more equal in strength. At the same time, boys report more minor forms of abuse while girls report higher levels of more severe forms of abuse. Abusers tend to be overly possessive, jealous and have mood swings. The victim often feels isolated from friends and family and bears the physical and emotional scars of the abuse (Offenhauer & Buchalter, 2011; O’keefe, 2005).
Romantic Relationships
According to the early psychologists like Freud, one of the major developmental tasks of adolescence is establishing romantic relationships with other youth (Furman & Shaffer, 2003). Erikson, Freud’s student, suggested however that adolescence is a time of identity search and adolescents will not be able to achieve mature romantic connections if their need for an identity is not met. Cross-cultural research has substantiated that most of the developmental goals that we construe for children and adolescents in the industrialized world are specific to the social needs in modern capitalist economies (Breyers & Seiffge-Krenke, 2010). Research has also demonstrated that many of the assumptions that youth hold about romance and sexual relationships are socially constructed and are highly gendered. Social sanctions around violating norms like monogamy or heterosexual behavior help keep these rules in place (Tolman & Tolman, 2009). In middle school, the social emphasis on boys controlling their relational behavior and emotions, especially in spaces like sports locker rooms, leads young boys to objectify girls and establish relationships of exploitation in heterosexual connections (Lorber & Farel, 1991).
Early adolescence is characterized by romantic relationships that are highly unstable. Going steady is often associated with superficial needs and there is frequent turnover of partners. Through adolescence, unlike friendships which often bring comfort to the young person, romantic alliances bring about heightened emotionality and intensifying conflict. Inexperience with these kinds of relationships adds to the intensity and ruminating nature of the connections. Unlike within friendships, the relatively high percentage of lack of reciprocity in feelings adds to the instability and the associated feelings of neglect and vulnerability. While overt norms in society value commitment and loyalty within romantic relationships, many adolescents are forced to deal with the conflicted feelings that come from violating these norms such as monogamy or honesty within relationships (Crouter & Booth, 2014).
All through their early socialization experiences, girls are more prepared to engage intimately with other people as compared to boys. Girls are taught to value relationships and are better prepared to disclose their authentic selves. Boys are socialized to hide their vulnerabilities and connect with other people by engaging in activities rather than by self-disclosure or strengthening their emotional connections. Tannen, Hamilton & Schiffrin (2015) refer to girls’ connection styles as being face to face and boys’ engagement style as being side by side. Studies have demonstrated that boys use very different language when connecting with their friends versus with their romantic connections. Girls on the other hand use the same engagement style and vocabulary with their friends and their romantic relationships. Boys have also learned, through socialization, that dominating over their partners is one way to establish their masculine credentials. During adolescence, girls struggle to be assertive within relationships whereas their male partners take their superiority for granted (Hertzog & Rowley, 2014; Honeycutt & Cantrill, 2014).
There has been little research on the influence of romantic relationships on the partners themselves and the conditions that foster good relationships. For many youths, especially those who are marginalized, being in a romantic relationship provides a way to feel good about themselves. However, these connections have downsides as well because of their instability and intensity (Machel, Disabato & Kashdan, 2016). There is some evidence that the movement to establish romantic connections in adolescence leads to a lowering of self-esteem, especially for girls. Dating during early adolescence is related to decreased academic achievement and lower levels of participation in school activities. Thus, while romantic connections can provide a safe space for some youth, it is often fleeting and by its very nature leads to emotional vulnerability (LaGreca & Harrison, 2005).
Studies indicate that dating at younger ages leads to earlier sexual experiences for both girls and boys (Bouchey & Furman, 2003). Engaging in cross-sex friendships earlier in life is related to earlier initiation into sex especially for boys (Connelly et al., 2004). Traditional notions of who engages in sex with whom has been upended by more recent work in the area. Youth do not just engage in sex with partners who they are in a dating relationship with. Surveys reveal that they also engage in sex with friends, previous partners who they have broken up with and with casual acquaintances. Under these circumstances, the importance of exploring safe sexual practices become even more important. Where there is greater asymmetry and difference between the partners (age, economic status, ethnicity) the lesser the frequency of use of condoms and engaging in safe sex (Center for Disease Control And Prevention, 2013). Two very salient factors that seem to predict lower contraceptive use is status asymmetry (when one has a higher status than another) and relationship asymmetry (when one partner is more invested in the relationship than the other). Girls who are engaged in a romantic relationship with an older man are some of the least likely to use contraception (Manning, Giordano & Longmore, 2006; Moore & Rosenthal, 1992; Troth & Peterson, 2000).
Sociological Frameworks Exploring Relationships
Current forms of adolescent friendships and social structures have their origin in the industrial revolution when the adult world was split from the social world of adolescents. In the modern context, youth are forced to congregate among themselves for long periods of time. While this has prevented them from assuming adult roles even when they are ready for it, it has allowed the space to form exclusive norms and a language to express their lived experiences that is their own – for example, engaging in collective silly action that most adults would label as infantile (Rotundo, 1993). Earlier theorists emphasized the notion that adolescent social structure derived their inspiration from intergenerational conflict and opposition to traditional order (Roseman, 2004). Today, that theory has been discarded; in fact, adolescents and adults in modern societies are more likely to be intergenerationally separated in a laissez faire relationship where they engage in social behavior that involves different spheres and spaces. Some experts have observed a lack of rebelliousness towards authority in today’s youth (Giancola, 2006). Many of our relationship experiences with family, peers and the larger community in contemporary societies can be understood by using the framework provided by Social Reproduction Theory or SRT.
Social Reproduction Theory (SRT)
The original ideas about social reproduction was introduced by Karl Marx. The central idea of SRT is that all goods and services are produced by workers and these must be continually manufactured. Workers who produce these goods and services must be reproduced as well even when they are not working. You need the reproduction of the working class both from one day to the next as well as from one generation to another for the system to be perpetuated. Since capitalists need a continuous stream of laborers, workers’ unpaid work outside of their paid labor must be harnessed to sustain them and the future generation of workers. This work is gendered, racialized and stratified by social class (Bhattacharya, 2017). Experts for instance have noted how most of the unpaid household needs and services are provided by women even as they are headed to paid labor in ever larger numbers. In an earlier era, communities were more intimately involved in raising children; today it has been privatized to the nuclear family, especially to the women in the family. Thus, on the surface, it may appear that the work sphere and the non-work spheres of workers’ lives are very different and unrelated to each other but when one digs deeper, the connections between the two become obvious. The conditions of non-work have a deep impact on the worker and vice versa. For instance, neighborhood quality affects work conditions while poor working conditions have an impact on the quality of living conditions outside of the working environment. Similarly, relationships within the school system as an institution have an impact on the future life of the children as workers and hence the reproduction of the social class system we are born into (Pluto Press, 2017; Sampson & Sharkey, 2008).
Social Reproduction Theory argues that schools are not meritocratic systems where children have equal opportunities to succeed but they are institutional spaces where old divisions based on social class, gender and race are perpetuated. There are several mechanisms through which this happens – within the curriculum by elevating some topics as being more important than others, expenditure of resources on various school activities that benefit middle and upper class children disproportionately, enforcement of exclusionary disciplinary processes that disadvantage children of color, and school rituals and sports that elevate values like competitiveness over cooperation, conformity and obedience over critical thinking and patriarchy. Social reproduction practices are woven into the very social fabric of schooling. Peer networks and relationships within traditional school spaces tend to support this hierarchy and bolster the unequal relations that exist between groups of students based on race, class, gender and other group identities (Sullivan et al., 2017; Thorne, 1993).
School Status and Maintenace
One way in which inequality is reproduced with each generation is through the maintenance of the social hierarchy in school. Research has found that concern over being popular peaks at about grade 9 in the USA. While the students at the top of the hierarchy often exert time and energy to maintaining their position within the system, they also actively avoid associating with lower status peers because the association might influence other students’ opinions about them (Eckert, 1989; Milner, 2013). Once the status of any student is established, it is relatively stable through adolescence. The established status is reinforced by the associations that students make and by feedback from authority figures. Occasionally, there is significant movement up or down the social ladder by a boy who may go through a sudden growth spurt or a girl who could possibly develop special talents but most times if there are status changes it is usually not very dramatic (Simmons, 2017). Students who find an opening to moving up the social ladder in school often find that they would have to distance themselves from their old friends and even put them down in order to be able to move up. The result is often that youth who were made fun of become downwardly mobile (Sentse, Kretschmer & Salmivalli, 2015). Marcel Danesi (1999) calls these insults small cruelties and adolescent school life is filled with them on a regular basis. Derogatory comments are often made by students when their object of derision is not present, and it forces other high-status group members to distance themselves from the lower status individuals. Coming up with a mocking nickname is another way that high status youth keep others in their place and influence their status. Such ritual ridicule is not restricted to verbal insults but also includes physical aggression where high status students would rough up someone of a lower status. Students of lower status thus have great incentive to avoid any contact with those who are of higher status in order to protect themselves physically and emotionally (Klein, 2006; Milner, 2013).
Much of the hostility, threats, ridicule and aggression arise from the way schools are organized where social and academic rewards are given only to a select few and most students experience little power in their interactions with peers, administrators and teachers. When the atmosphere is wrought with hostile feelings, aggression is used as an instrument to gain status. Students practice their put-downs with students’ lower down in the ladder with few consequences. The put-downs and insults are also used to keep people from deviating from what is considered normal behavior in the dominant group by making the consequences intolerable for those most vulnerable to insults. The result is often that those who enjoy high status are those who come from the most privileged segments of the social economic, political, gender, racial and religious hierarchy. Clearly, there are also more obvious ways in which children coming from families of privilege advance within the educational system as demonstrated in the recent example where a sting operation found many wealthy families bribing their way into elite colleges (PBS Newshour, 2019).
Fostering inter-ethnic friendships
Most children in the USA will be of non-European origin by the year 2040. A significant proportion of them will have at least one immigrant parent (Passel & Cohn, 2008). Schools can provide important spaces for children and youth to meet students from other cultural and social contexts where they can learn diverse frameworks for viewing the world. Encouraging friendships across social boundaries can help reduce prejudice and further understanding. Teachers, school counselors and administrators can help advance the cultural competence of young people by facilitating better communication among students. Studies demonstrate that better intergroup relations predict higher academic achievement, and social emotional development for all students (Kawabata & Crick, 2008). In schools where race and the social hierarchy of the school coincide, intergroup relationships are harder to form and sustain. Thus, fostering cross-cultural friendships and associations involves questioning the inequities that exist in schools (Graham, Munniksma & Juvonen, 2014).
From early childhood onwards, children tend to have friends from within their own racial group and intragroup friendships are rated more highly than intergroup friendships. This phenomenon seems to be especially strong for white children who tend to have fewer friends from other ethnic groups. Where there is active effort made on the part of the adults in a child’s life to foster varied connections with children from diverse communities, the results are often dramatic prejudice reduction, higher self-efficacy, and appreciation of other cultures. The chance to form friendships across racial and ethnic lines allows for young people to understand others as individuals and thereby resist stereotypes (Sigelman & Welch, 1993).
Allport (1954) hypothesized that for optimal friendships between different ethnic groups in a school context they must feel equal, have common goals and have the support of administrators and other adults. It is not just enough to have diversity among the student body; it must be supported by the institutional framework. Some ways to assist in facilitating interethnic contact and friendships include a) Mixed group collaborative projects with shared goals; b) Having open discussions about race, racism and other forms of prejudice in both formal and informal settings to further the goals of honest communication between students; c) Having an inclusive curriculum with regards to race, gender, sexual orientation and disability studies; d) Incorporating informal conversations on prejudice reduction and equity. Research studies have shown that facilitating direct conversations between students from varied backgrounds in terms of race, ethnicity, social class, religion, abilities, gender identity and sexual orientations have had the greatest impact in prejudice reduction, self-awareness, and appreciation of diversity (Brewer & Gaertner, 2004).
One of the main factors that prevents intergroup friendships is the systemic barriers that exist in schools that do not allow for engagement across social class lines. Often, extracurricular activities may cost family resources that may not be affordable or may be scheduled at times when the student cannot be there. Thought must be given to alleviate these structural barriers. Data on discipline, punishment and school activities and achievement should be consistently collected in order to identify areas of improvement with regard to equity. The bottom line is that it is difficult to foster intergroup communication and connection when there is deep inequality in the social and academic fabric of the school life. Overcoming the structural barriers that keep intergroup divisions in place requires all the various institutions in society from governmental agencies, schools, communities and families to engage with each other in a constructive manner.
Review and Reflect
REVIEW
1.What are the mechanisms through which divisions based on social class, gender and race are perpetuated in schools?
2.What are the roles of teachers, school counselors and administrators in fostering interethnic relationships between students at schools?
REFLECT
1.As an adolescent, what was your school status? How did you maintain or seek to change it and why?
Issues Related to Relationships and Intimacy
Adolescence is a time of changing relationships and the new needs that arise from increased maturity and the developmental tasks that society sets forth for youth. As we saw in the previous section, youths at this age experience intense relationships focused on shared goals and needs for intimacy and loyalty. One’s developing sense of identity forces them to seek friends and peers who will facilitate and mirror who the young person is. The challenges that arise from these yearnings must be dealt with and addressed by both the adults in the young person’s life as well as the institutions that that the individual functions within, i.e., schools and family.
Cyber-bullying
There has been an explosion of research on cyberbullying in recent years. Most experts agree that the presence of technology has led to an increase in cyberbullying among youth. Children today can handle technology in complex ways earning them the nickname Digital Natives (Prensky, 2001). Many of these youth have never known a time when there was no digital technology. This contrasts with the Digital Immigrants who grew up during a time when technological devices were absent or were not easily available. While much of this new access to technology has proven beneficial to young people giving them diverse ways to get information, providing them new avenues to be creative and allowing them easy connectivity to friends and peers, youth are often unschooled about the dark side of cyber use. The new digital forms of bullying are perhaps some of the most destructive ways in which a small number of young people use social media and other online tools.
Olweus, Limber & Mihalic (1999) define bullying as an action where there is intentional harm towards the victim, the harmful behavior is repeatedly performed, and there is a power imbalance between the victim and the bully. Smith et al (2008) define cyberbullying as “an aggressive intentional act carried out by a group or individuals, using electronic forms of contact, repeatedly and over time, against a victim who cannot easily defend himself or herself” (p.376). Whatever the form of bullying, the phenomenon causes core distress among victims, occurs when there is no supervision, starts in school and spills over outside of school, and the victim usually knows the bully. A crucial difference between cyberbullying and face-to-face bullying is that in the former case, it is difficult for the bully to see the victim’s distress, which often makes it easy to be uninhibited in the problematic behavior. Some experts argue that cyberbullying also robs the bully of the pleasure of seeing another person suffer. Another difference between the two forms of bullying is that of anonymity – in cyberbullying the victim often does not know who is doing the bullying. This emboldens individuals who may not otherwise use bullying to resolve an issue. How the bullying behavior is replicated also distinguishes between both forms of bullying. In cyberspace, a simple act of aggression by one bully may be repeated endlessly by others uninvolved in the original incident by sending forward an email or a picture or re-posting threatening messages. Thus, the consequences of cyberbullying on all the students involved may be longer-lasting. One third of bullies both online and face-to-face are themselves victims of bullying (Haynie et al., 2001).
There are gender differences in the kind of bullying youth are subjected to. Girls use indirect/relational forms of bullying more frequently while boys engage in more direct/physical aggression (Rivers & Smith, 1994). Relational aggression involves verbal and emotional harassment of individuals repeatedly and with the intention to do harm. Some cyberbullying experts claim that girls are more likely to engage in online forms of aggression because of the increased likelihood of girls engaging in relational aggression (Crick & Grotpeter, 1995). The internet lends itself easily to relational violence. However, other studies do not show a gender difference in cyber aggression (Li, 2006). Several studies have demonstrated that girls report more negative impacts and are more likely to experience exclusion and revelation of private personal information as a violation of trust (Smith et al., 2008).
The consequences of bullying in the victim tends to be depression, poor self-esteem, anxiety, suicidal ideation, psychosomatic problems like headaches and sleep disturbances, low academic achievement, not feeling safe in school and a risk for problems at school (Hoertel et al., 2011; Meyer, 2015). The effects of cyberbullying may be intensified by its very nature; most youth have difficulty cutting oneself off from technology because of the social isolation it produces even when they are bullied. The public nature of the aggression online, not knowing who has seen the bullying posts and the permanent nature of the posts themselves leading to repeated exposure to the same messages make the encounters traumatic for individuals.
Lynn Mikel Brown writes that bully-prevention has become a huge for-profit business with top-down strategies aimed at reducing bullying behavior in school. She suggests that the solutions are often simple with the adults in children’s lives taking responsibility for the unsafe environments that children are forced to spend hours in. She offers 10 simple strategies to address behaviors that form bullying. Strategies for addressing bullying include:
Stop labeling kids as bullies and victims: Children are complex and are capable of both doing good and harm to others. Adults ought to take responsibility if schools are unsafe.
Name the behavior and not the child: Identifying the specific behavior like homophobia or sexual harassment or ableism is more productive to pointing to a consequence or a strategy than simply calling it bullying.
Move beyond the individual: Examine social factors like racism, classism, homophobia and ableism as sources of aggressive behaviors. How does cultural, social and economic capital affect bullying behavior in school?
One size does not fit all: While Olweus programs have worked in Norway, they may not show the same effectiveness in diverse schools in the United States.
Have realistic expectations for students: Expecting students to adhere to standards that adults may find difficult to follow is unrealistic. Having students always follow rules imposed from above, never resisting or fighting back when their rights are violated, are counterproductive in reducing aggressive behaviors.
Listen to students: Understanding where kids come from and their perspective helps undermine the damaging impact of privilege and protects the less powerful. Often, aggression stems from an unequal status quo.
Embrace Grassroots Movement: Change has to be student-driven, not enforced top-down for effectiveness. Including students in dialogues about school rules increases compliance by students.
Be Proactive/Not Reactive: Working towards making schools safe for all students helps in reducing bullying. Clubs like Civil Rights Team Projects allow students who are otherwise marginalized in schools to explore their identity.
Coalition Building: Working towards building bridges between different groups allows for marginalized youth to find allies and respond to violence in constructive ways.
Accentuate the Positive: Adult advocates for youth must recognize that youth need more control over their lives to feel important.
One area that has not been explored well is the influence of peer groups in both the bullying behavior as well as an effective means of stopping the bullying. There is evidence that children who are willing to intervene when a peer is being bullied report greater empathy than those who do not (Cowie & Olafsson, 2000). However, peer intervention is influenced by the social standing of the bully or bullies, the number of people engaged in the bullying incidence, and the relationship of the bystander to the bully as well as the one being bullied. Thus, peers may be reluctant to intervene if there are several, more popular peers who are the bullies or if the victim is unpopular (Rodkin & Hodges, 2003). The larger climate in peer communities around issues of race, gender and sexual orientation influence bullying behaviors. Rarely does prejudiced aggression take place in isolation but often, the community normalizes certain forms of prejudice which is then exhibited as physical or emotional violence against individuals from underrepresented groups. As many experts observe, it is hard to prevent incidents of bullying if diversity and inclusion are not infused into the school curriculum and it its rituals.
Sexuality Education
From its very origins, sexuality education in the US had a rather ambivalent framework that was mostly aimed at keeping youth informed about the negative consequences of engaging in sex while stifling a more scientific understanding of the topic. The earliest sex education programs addressed the issues of chastity, nutrition and the evils of masturbation. In their presentations of the topic, early educators connected masturbation to warts, constipation, loss of energy, insanity and death. In the late 1800s when the first sex education programs were launched, the Catholic church tried to shut them down. During World War I, the country experienced widespread sexually transmitted diseases (STD) and infections (STIs) and this prompted the first organized sexuality education programs within the military. Later, the curriculum was adopted by schools to teach about STIs and hygiene. In 1964, MaryCalderone, a physician at Planned Parenthood, established the Sexuality Information and Education Council of the USA (SIECUS). The organization attempted to move away from scare tactics and teach sexuality from a scientific viewpoint. It also established the first programs to train teachers in sexuality education methods. Religious conservatives attacked SIECUS and claimed that sex education had the potential to further the cause of communism and create chaos. Rumors were spread that sexuality educators were stripping themselves in front of their students. In 1994, the surgeon general of the US was forced to resign after she admitted that masturbation can reduce unsafe sexual activity (Middleton, 1998; Pardini, 2002). To this day, the country still struggles to form a coherent policy on sexuality education for young people.
Current sexuality education in schools often replicate relationship of hegemonic sexuality and heterosexism, reinforcing racist and classist stereotypes. While the debate about the efficacy of abstinence education is centered around whether teaching abstinence leads to lower or higher engagement with safe sex, there is very little discussion around homophobia, racism and classism. Some authors argue that sex education in schools normalizes inequality (Ferfolja, 2007; Fields, 2008). Conservative groups initially resisted all forms of sex education but the advent of HIV/AIDS in the 1980s forced these groups to shift tactic and advocate for abstinence-only education. During the HIV pandemic, conservatives encouraged more resources to be set aside for abstinence education. In abstinence only education, teens are encouraged to wait until marriage to engage in monogamous sex. By 2005, the dominant model for educating teens about sex was abstinence only education, though in recent years there has been a shift to ‘abstinence + condom’ where abstinence is still held up as the only appropriate sexuality related behavior (Perrin & DeJoy, 2003). On both sides of the issue, children have been constructed as victims of sex; thus, education around sexuality is often based on inculcating fear in children. White teens are especially constructed as ‘innocent’ and needing to be protected while youth of color are perceived as being hypersexualized and in need of restraint (Chan et al, 2001). Today, many states have the provision that public schools will be stripped of funding for sex education if they taught comprehensive sexuality education.
Gays, lesbians, bisexual, transsexuals, and queers are invisible in these lessons and six states in the USA require instructors to label behavior associated with non-normative sexual behaviors as being self and other destructive (Fields, 2008). Even when students seek out information on non-normative sexualities, they are prevented from gaining accurate information. Another missing element from sexuality education in schools is an exploration of female desire. Girls are portrayed as victims that need protection with little discussion about their own needs and desires (Garcia, 2009). All these misconceptions about sexuality and youth lead to large public acceptance of rape myths that objectify girls and women and construct boys as people with insatiable sexual appetite.
Schools teach children implicitly about how to do gender and sexuality through various curriculum but especially when it relates to sexuality education. Homophobia and heterosexist messages prevail from elementary school to high school. In high schools, through the many rituals during the school year, boys learn how to do masculinity and learn an aversion to feminine behavior (Pascoe, 2007). Rituals like the prom and sporting events reinforce and reward hierarchical heterosexism. Sexually harassing behaviors, sexist language and homophobic slurs frequently used by students are ignored by teachers thereby reinforcing inequality based on gender. Similar behaviors by Black and white students are perceived very differently by administrators – Black students’ behaviors are framed as sexually loaded while white student behaviors are ignored as harmless, thereby hypersexualizing Black students, especially boys (Ferguson, 2000). It is harder for teens to abstain from sexual behavior in a society where desire floods popular media and images consistently communicate hypersexuality and sexual objectification.
A critical pedagogy of sexuality education can overcome some of the deepest concerns about traditional sex education. In order for an equity based sexuality pedagogy instructors must use strong anti-discriminatory language when discussing and introducing sex education. This will go a long way towards an equal education for all groups. The information needs to be medically accurate instead of simply trying to induce fear in young people. Teachers would need to be sensitive to issues of race, class, gender and sexual orientation and they ought to be ready to acknowledge adolescent desires. Last but not least, schools should focus on peer education; this could make students feel safe about expressing themselves in an authentic manner.
These alternative pedagogies could be fraught with their own pitfalls – homosexuality may become central to the discussion marginalizing other groups, and teachers may let their own prejudices get in the way of effective teaching. Apart from the formal education about sex that adolescents receive, there is also an informal curriculum with peers teaching each other about how to do sex and gender. Much of this learning is hierarchical. Beauty magazines and clothing provide props to discuss heterosexual relationships and they solidify class and race divisions among youth. Sexual performance and sexual restraint help reinforce stereotypes about teens based on race and class. Among boys, heterosexism is reinforced by objectification of girls, homophobic language, and boasting about heterosexual connections made. However, peers also have the potential to disrupt traditional ideas about sexuality and gender. Parents reinforce traditional gender norms and heterosexism by emphasizing the safety aspect of sex and discriminating between girls and boys in how much sexual freedom a child is allowed. Fathers are especially likely to express homophobic ideas and focus on heterosexual relationship when talking about sex in adolescence. Many parents experience resistance among their children when they do attempt to talk about sex especially if they are themselves ambivalent about the topic. Teachers and other advocates can help provide safe spaces for youth to challenge and transform their traditional frameworks for comprehending gendered and sexual behavior.
Coping with Loss and Grieving
Many people assume that grieving in adolescence takes on similar characteristics as adults coping with deep loss. However, research has demonstrated that adult grief exhibits a different set of signs as compared to youth grief. As discussed earlier, teens are very peer oriented. When they do face loss, it often tends to be sudden and traumatic since few youths die of natural causes. The top reasons for youth mortality are auto accidents, suicides and homicides. There is little warning preceding the loss and this leads to young people not feeling safe in their communities after the loss of their loved one. The response to traumatic loss is severe in young people and is marked by greater functional impairment, suicidal ideation and physical disability. The move to be independent from families and other adults predisposes adolescents to not communicate their emotional states to the adults that they interact with every day. Hence, it is difficult for teachers and parents to gauge how a sudden loss has affected the individual concerned. Youth are often concerned that they may be judged harshly by their peers if they expressed their fears, anxieties and sadness. This leads to self-silencing of their negative emotions. They may feel very alone in their misery since they feel unique and different from other people. Teens may express their sadness through spending hours listening to music or exercising or even engaging in destructive behavior (Wingate, 2017). They may feel more comfortable expressing their sadness online in an anonymous fashion and while this may provide some relief, they may also be subjected to misinformation and cruel taunts. Unlike children, adolescents may not be able to rely on simple answers from their religious faith to comfort them since they may be moving away from their spiritual or religious traditions and questioning cherished family beliefs (Cox & Stevenson, 2017; Sofka, 2009).
Adolescents who may be experiencing profound transformations in their identity or who may be questioning their identity intensely may be even more affected by general questions around death and specific anxieties about the loss of a loved one. While youth are questioning the meaning of their own lives and the purpose of their actions in the larger scheme of things, death may make their lives appear unstable, unpredictable and scary. Problem-focused and emotion regulation only appear later in adolescence making responses to loss extreme and sometimes destructive in young people (Doka, 2017).
Grief in teens may be unacknowledged because of the nature of the loss – abortions, the loss of a loved pet, perinatal loss, incarceration of a loved one or a parent, divorce, placement in a new foster home (Doka, 2017). The grief of children and youth are left unacknowledged because they are thought to be incapable of feeling to the same intensity as adults. In some cases, the circumstances of the loss may be deeply shameful and therefore unacknowledged. Children and teens often experience the loss of a parent they have not had contact with deeply because of the lost time and opportunity to establish a connection. The way adults respond to their children’s grief may teach young people lifelong lessons in how to cope with loss and sorrow. Thus, listening respectfully as youth voice their feelings gives everybody an opportunity to strengthen their connections while also learning appropriate ways of coping (Moore, 2018). Brushing the feelings aside and pretending that they do not matter sets up patterns that prevent conflict resolution that youth take with them into adulthood.
In order to help youth, educational institutions would need to have clear policies and guidelines to help grieving students cope with schoolwork and their lives at school. A death of a family member especially by homicide or suicide often brings with grief, a sense of shame. They may be treated differently by other people or there may be silence around the event that the adolescent may feel is embarrassing. Teachers, staff and administrators would have to be trained to look for signs of grieving in students who may be acting out or whose grades have dropped precipitously. Setting up helping groups might facilitate the participants sharing of their thoughts and feelings about the events that may be bothering them. Some organizations offer grief camps for children and teens to find solace as they cope with their loss. Comfort Zone Camp and Camp Erin are two such camps that have national reach. They have an online profile as well so students can continue their healing process even after camp ends. Hospice groups offer grief counseling and healing groups as well (Warnick, 2015).
The cyber environment that adolescents immerse themselves in provides a natural outlet to adolescents as they face loss. Online environments tend to be more participatory and have fewer barriers to engagement and artistic expression. It gives youth a little more control over how and when they participate, and the feedback is often immediate. Thus, even as they face marginalization and a sense of ‘difference’ at a time of profound loss, the online environment may provide a more accepting space for a grieving teen (Williams & Merton, 2009). Youth and young adults have built shrines to commemorate the memories of their loved ones; there are several sites that offer easy templates that youth then fill in with digital artefacts. Some popular sites include cemetery.org, teensremembered.org, and worldgardens.com. Many adolescents are using web logs to record their experiences with illness, loss and coping with death. Facebook and other social media websites allow for messages of condolences and organizing of vigils quickly. The challenge for a moderator of any social media platform is to keep the remembrance respectful and positive. It is also important to educate teens about being sensitive to their friends needing help, especially after a suicide event. Ultimately, both digital immigrants and digital natives, adults and children, advocates for youth and the youth themselves need to learn about the technologies that are available for use during times of stress and grief. At the same time, it is important to teach adolescents how to keep themselves safe when they reach out for support in a digital environment (Balk, Zaengle & Carr, 2011; Sofka, 2009).
Conclusion
In summary, adolescence is a time of both reaching out and pulling back, a time of increasing autonomy but at the same time strengthening ties at school and in one’s home. Youth’s social lives become increasingly complex and social inequalities acquire even more salience as individuals in the age group try to negotiate power and intimacy within complex environments. During adolescence, youth acquire the capacity to emotionally self-regulate even as they face challenges that relate to romance and intimacy. Adults play a crucial role in helping young people successfully name and validate their own feelings even as they develop the capacity for empathy and compassion towards others.
Review and Reflect
REVIEW
1.What are some of the main differences between face-to-face and cyber bullying?
2.What are some of the gender differences in the kind of bullying youth are subjected to?
3.What are the differences between traditional sex education practices and critical pedagogy around issues of sexuality education?
4.How can educational institutions help youth as they cope with grief and loss?
REFLECT
1.Did you personally experience bullying or had a close friend who did? What was that experience like? Was there support from the school around remedying the situation? What role did adults play in remedying the situation?
Glossary of Terms
‘digital immigrants’
‘digital natives’
‘marshmallow experiments’
ableism
accentuate the positive
active problem-solving strategies
adult grief exhibits a different set of signs as compared to youth grief
be proactive/ not reactive
bullying
classism
cliques
coalition building
critical pedagogy
crowd
cyberbullying
direct/physical
embrace grassroots movement
emotional intelligence
empathy
face to face
have realistic expectations for students
homophobia
indirect/relational
internally focused emotional control
language and ritualized behavior
listen to students
name the behavior
one size does not fit all
racism
ritual ridicule
self-regulation
self-silencing of their negative emotions
sexual harassment
side by side
small cruelties
social referencing
stop labeling kids
References
Alba, R. D., & Nee, R. (2003). Remaking the American Mainstream: Assimilation and the New Immigration. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Perseus Books.
Anderson, S., & Leventhal, T. (2017). Residential mobility and adolescent achievement and behavior: Understanding timing and extent of mobility. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 27(2), 328-343.
Balk, D. E., Zaengle, D., & Corr, C. A. (2011). Strengthening grief support for adolescents coping with a peer’s death. School Psychology International, 32(2), 144-162.
Bengtson, V. L. (2001). Beyond the nuclear family: The increasing importance of multigenerational bonds. Journal of marriage and family, 63(1), 1-16.
Besharov, D. J., & Gardiner, K. N. (1993). Teen sex: truth and consequences. American Enterprise, 4(1), 52-59.
Bhattacharya, T. (2017). Social Reproduction Theory: Remapping Class, Recentering Oppression
Boiché, J., Sarrazin, P., & Chanal, J. (2015). A longitudinal study of perceived conflict and instrumental relationships between life contexts among adolescents: The role of self-determined motivation. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 27(4), 430-448.
Bouchey, H. A., & Furman, W. (2003). Dating and romantic experiences in adolescence. Blackwell handbook of adolescence, 312-329.
Brewer, M. B., & Gaertner, S. L. (2004). Toward reduction of prejudice: Intergroup contact and social categorization. Self and social identity, 298, 318.
Beyers, W., & Seiffge-Krenke, I. (2010). Does Identity Precede Intimacy? Testing Erikson’s Theory on Romantic Development in Emerging Adults of the 21st Century. Journal of Adolescent Research, 25(3), 387–415.
Brown, B. B., & Klute, C. (2003). Friendships, cliques, and crowds.
Brown, L.M. (2009). Ten ways to move beyond bully-prevention. Rethinking Schools.
Buhrmester, D., & Furman, W. (1987). The development of companionship and intimacy. Child development, 1101-1113.
Calarco, J, (2018). “Why Rich Kids Are So Good at the Marshmallow Test”. The Atlantic.
Center For Disease Control And Prevention (2013). Beyond the Data: Reducing Teen Pregnancy. Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=1-8YyP5N8-w
Clark, M. L., & Ayers, M. (1992). Friendship similarity during early adolescence: Gender and racial patterns. The Journal of psychology, 126(4), 393-405.
Collier, G., & Collier, G. J. (2014). Emotional expression. Psychology Press.
Connolly, J., Craig, W., Goldberg, A., & Pepler, D. (2004). Mixed‐gender groups, dating, and romantic relationships in early adolescence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 14(2), 185-207.
Connolly, J., & McIsaac, C. (2011). Romantic relationships in adolescence. Social development: Relationships in infancy, childhood, and adolescence, 3, 180-203.
Coontz, S. (2015). Romance and sex in adolescence and emerging adulthood. In Romance and sex in adolescence and emerging adulthood (pp. 115-120). Routledge.
Cox, G. R., & Stevenson, R. G. (2017). Children, Adolescents, and Death: Questions and Answers. Routledge.
Cowie, H., & Olafsson, R. (2000). The role of peer support in helping the victims of bullying in a school with high levels of aggression. School psychology international, 21(1), 79-95.
Cutting, A. L., & Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of mind, emotion understanding, language, and family background: Individual differences and interrelations. Child development, 70(4), 853-865.
Davis, K. (2012). Friendship 2.0: Adolescents’ experiences of belonging and self-disclosure online. Journal of adolescence, 35(6), 1527-1536.
Danesi, M. (1999). Cool: The Signs and Meanings of Adolescence. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.
Diamond, J. (2012). The World Until Yesterday. Penguin Inc.: New York, USA.
Denham, S. A. (1998). Emotional development in young children. Guilford Press.
Diener, M. L., & Mangelsdorf, S. C. (1999). Behavioral strategies for emotion regulation in toddlers: Associations with maternal involvement and emotional expressions. Infant Behavior and Development, 22(4), 569-583.
Doka, K. J. (2017). The Disenfranchised Grief of Children and Adolescents. Children, Adolescents, and Death: Questions and Answers, 37.
Eckert, P. (1989). Jocks and burnouts: Social categories and identity in high school. New York: Columbia University, Teachers College
Eggum, N. D., Eisenberg, N., Kao, K., Spinrad, T. L., Bolnick, R., Hofer, C., … & Fabricius, W. V. (2011). Emotion understanding, theory of mind, and prosocial orientation: Relations over time in early childhood. The journal of positive psychology, 6(1), 4-16.
Faris, R., & Felmlee, D. H. (2018). Best Friends for Now: Friendship Network Stability and Adolescents’ Life Course Goals. In Social Networks and the Life Course (pp. 185-203). Springer, Cham.
Ferfolja, T. (2007). Schooling cultures: Institutionalizing heteronormativity and heterosexism. International journal of inclusive education, 11(2), 147-162.
Fields, J. (2008). Risky lessons: Sex education and social inequality. Rutgers University Press.
Fortuin, J., van Geel, M., & Vedder, P. (2015). Peer influences on internalizing and externalizing problems among adolescents: A longitudinal social network analysis. Journal of youth and adolescence, 44(4), 887-897.
Furman, W., Brown, B. B., & Feiring, C. (Eds.). (1999). The development of romantic relationships in adolescence. Cambridge University Press
Gardner, T. W., Dishion, T. J., & Connell, A. M. (2008). Adolescent self-regulation as resilience: Resistance to antisocial behavior within the deviant peer context. Journal of abnormal child psychology, 36(2), 273-284.
Garner, P. W. (1995). Toddlers’ emotion regulation behaviors: The roles of social context and family expressiveness. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 156(4), 417-430.
George, M. J., & Odgers, C. L. (2015). Seven fears and the science of how mobile technologies may be influencing adolescents in the digital age. Perspectives on psychological science, 10(6), 832-851.
Gestsdóttir, S., & Lerner, R. M. (2007). Intentional self-regulation and positive youth development in early adolescence: Findings from the 4-h study of positive youth development. Developmental psychology, 43(2), 508.
Gifford-Smith, M., Dodge, K. A., Dishion, T. J., & McCord, J. (2005). Peer influence in children and adolescents: Crossing the bridge from developmental to intervention science. Journal of abnormal child psychology, 33(3), 255-265.
Giancola, F. (2006). The generation gap: More myth than reality. People and strategy, 29(4), 32.
Giordano, P. C., Cernkovich, S. A., & DeMaris, A. (1993). The family and peer relations of Black adolescents. Journal of Marriage and Family, 55(2), 277.
Gleason, T., Gower, A., Hohmann, L., & Gleason, T. (2005). Temperament and friendship in preschool-aged children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29(4), 336-344.
Goodwin, M. H. (2006). The hidden life of girls: Games of stance, status, and exclusion (Vol. 1). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Gordon, M. M. (1964). Assimilation in American Life: The Role of Race, Religion, and National Origins. New York: Oxford University Press.
Graham, J. A., Cohen, R., Zbikowski, S. M., & Secrist, M. E. (1998). A longitudinal investigation of race and sex as factors in children’s classroom friendship choices. Child Study Journal, 28(4), 245-245.
Greenberg, M. T. (2006). Promoting resilience in children and youth. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094(1), 139-150.
Hartup, W. W. (1993). Adolescents and their friends. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 1993(60), 3-22.
Haynie, D. L., Nansel, T., Eitel, P., Crump, A. D., Saylor, K., Yu, K., & Simons-Morton, B. (2001). Bullies, victims, and bully/victims: Distinct groups of at-risk youth. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 21(1), 29-49.
Helsen, M., Vollebergh, W., & Meeus, W. (2000). Social support from parents and friends and emotional problems in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 29(3), 319-335.
Hertzog, J. L., & Rowley, R. L. (2014). My beliefs of my peers’ beliefs: Exploring the gendered nature of social norms in adolescent romantic relationships. Journal of interpersonal violence, 29(2), 348-368.
Hey, V. (1997). The company she keeps: An ethnography of girls’ friendships. McGraw-Hill Education (UK).
Hoertel, N., Strat, Y. L., Lavaud, P., & Limosin, F. (2011). Gender effects in bullying: Results from a national sample. Psychiatry Research, 200(2-3), 921-927.
Hoffman, M. L. (2001). Empathy and moral development: Implications for caring and justice. Cambridge University Press.
Hoffman, M. L. (1991). Empathy, social cognition, and moral action. William L. Kurtines/Jack L. Gewirtz: Handbook of moral behavior and development. Bd, 1, 275-301.
Honeycutt, J. M., & Cantrill, J. G. (2014). Cognition, communication, and romantic relationships. Routledge.
Hospice Of The Chesapeake (2016). Grief Out Loud: teens Talk About Loss. Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=qgrRoJyljeQ
Johnson, S. C., Baxter, L. C., Wilder, L. S., Pipe, J. G., Heiserman, J. E., & Prigatano, G. P. (2002). Neural correlates of self‐reflection. Brain, 125(8), 1808-1814.
Kawabata, Y., & Crick, N. R. (2008). The role of cross-racial/ethnic friendships in social adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 44(4), 1177.
Kenny, U., O’Malley-Keighran, M. P., Molcho, M., & Kelly, C. (2017). Peer Influences on Adolescent Body Image: Friends or Foes?. Journal of Adolescent Research, 32(6), 768-799.
Kit Welchlin (2014). What does self-regulation have to do with emotional intelligence. Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=dHaUVbCY9xs
Klein, J. (2006). Cultural capital and high school bullies: How social inequality impacts school violence. Men and Masculinities, 9(1), 53-75.
La Greca, A. M., & Harrison, H. M. (2005). Adolescent peer relations, friendships, and romantic relationships: Do they predict social anxiety and depression?. Journal of clinical child and adolescent psychology, 34(1), 49-61.
Larson, R., & Asmussen, L. (1991). Anger, worry, and hurt in early adolescence: An enlarging world of negative emotions. Adolescent stress: Causes and consequences, 21-41.
Larson, R. W., & Verma, S. (1999). How children and adolescents spend time across the world: work, play, and developmental opportunities. Psychological bulletin, 125(6), 701.
Li, Q. (2006). Cyberbullying in schools: A research of gender differences. School psychology international, 27(2), 157-170.
Lorber, J., & Farrell, S. A. (Eds.). (1991). The social construction of gender (pp. 313-337). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Lewis, P., & Simpson, R. (2007). Gender and emotions: introduction.
Lutz, C. A. (1996). Engendered Emotion: Gender, Power, and the Rhetoric of Emotional Control. The emotions: Social, cultural and biological dimensions, 151.
Machell, K. A., Disabato, D. J., & Kashdan, T. B. (2016). Buffering the negative impact of poverty on youth: The power of purpose in life. Social Indicators Research, 126(2), 845-861.
Manning, W. D., Giordano, P. C., & Longmore, M. A. (2006). Hooking up: The relationship contexts of “nonrelationship” sex. Journal of Adolescent Research, 21(5), 459-483.
Meyer, E. J. (2015). Gender, bullying, and harassment: Strategies to end sexism and homophobia in schools. Teachers College Press.
Middleton, S. (1998). Disciplining Sexuality Foucault, Life Histories, and Education.
Miller, B. C., & Benson, B. (1999). Romantic and sexual relationship development during adolescence. The development of romantic relationships in adolescence, 99-121.
Milner, M. (2013). Freaks, geeks, and cool kids. Routledge.
Mischel, W; Shoda, Y; Rodriguzez, M L. (1989). “Delay of gratification in children”. Science. 244 (4907): 933–938.
Moore, C. (2018). Helping Grieving Children and Adolescents. In Clinical Handbook of Bereavement and Grief Reactions (pp. 189-215). Humana Press, Cham
Moore, S., & Rosenthal, D. (1992). The social context of adolescent sexuality: safe sex implications. Journal of Adolescence, 15(4), 415-435.
Offenhauer, P., & Buchalter, A. (2011, June). Teen dating violence: A literature review and annotated bibliography. In Library of Congress–Federal Research Division.
O’Keefe, M. (2005). Teen dating violence: A review of risk factors and prevention efforts. National Electronic Network on violence against women, 1, 1-5.
Olweus, D., Limber, S., & Mihalic, S. F. (1999). Blueprints for violence prevention, book nine: Bullying prevention program. Boulder, CO: Center for the Study and Prevention of Violence.
Pardini, P. (2002). The history of sexuality education. Rethinking schools online, 12(4).
PBS newshour (2019). How a bombshell bribery scandal illuminates the corruption of college admissions. Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=epZeq_SJ6cI
Passel, J. S., & Cohn, D. V. U. S. (2008). US population projections: 2005-2050.
Perrin, K. K., & DeJoy, S. B. (2003). Abstinence-only education: How we got here and where we’re going. Journal of public health policy, 24(3-4), 445-459.
Pluto Press (2017). What is Social Reproduction Theory? Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=Uur-pMk7XjY
Prensky, M. (2001). Digital natives, digital immigrants part 1. On the horizon, 9(5), 1-6.
Riediger, M., & Klipker, K. (2014). Emotion regulation in adolescence. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation. 2nd ed., pp. 187–202). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Rivers, I., & Smith, P. K. (1994). Types of bullying behaviour and their correlates. Aggressive behavior, 20(5), 359-368.
Rodkin, P. C., & Hodges, E. V. (2003). Bullies and victims in the peer ecology: Four questions for psychologists and school professionals. School Psychology Review, 32(3), 384-400.
Rood, L., Roelofs, J., Bögels, S. M., & Arntz, A. (2012). The effects of experimentally induced rumination, positive reappraisal, acceptance, and distancing when thinking about a stressful event on affect states in adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40(1), 73–84.
Rothbart, M. K., Ziaie, H., & O’boyle, C. G. (1992). Self‐regulation and emotion in infancy. New directions for child and adolescent development, 1992(55), 7-23.
Rotundo, E. A. (1993). American manhood: Transformations in masculinity from the revolution to the modern era. Basic Books.
Rotz, D. (2016). Why have divorce rates fallen?: the role of women’s age at marriage. Journal of Human Resources, 51(4), 961-1002.
Rubin, K. H., & Mills, R. S. (1988). The many faces of social isolation in childhood. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology, 56(6), 916.
Rulison, K. L., Gest, S. D., & Osgood, D. W. (2015). Adolescent peer networks and the potential for the diffusion of intervention effects. Prevention Science, 16(1), 133-144.
Sampson, R. J., & Sharkey, P. (2008). Neighborhood selection and the social reproduction of concentrated racial inequality. Demography, 45(1), 1-29.
Seidman, E., Chesir-Teran, D., Friedman, J. L., Yoshikawa, H., Allen, L., Roberts, A., & Aber, J. L. (1999). The risk and protective functions of perceived family and peer microsystems among urban adolescents in poverty. American Journal of Community Psychology, 27(2), 211-237.
Sentse, M., Kretschmer, T., & Salmivalli, C. (2015). The longitudinal interplay between bullying, victimization, and social status: Age‐related and gender differences. Social Development, 24(3), 659-677
Shapiro, L. E. (1997). How to raise a child with a high EQ: A parent’s guide to emotional intelligence. HarperCollins Publishers.
Shulman, S., Levy‐Shiff, R., Kedem, P., & Alon, E. (1997). Intimate relationships among adolescent romantic partners and same‐sex friends: Individual and systemic perspectives. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 1997(78), 37-51.
Sigelman, L., & Welch, S. (1993). The contact hypothesis revisited: Black-white interaction and positive racial attitudes. Social forces, 71(3), 781-795.
Simmons, R. G. (2017). Moving into adolescence: The impact of pubertal change and school context. Routledge.
Siu, A. M. H., & Shek, D. T. L. (2010). Social problem solving as a predictor of well-being in adolescents and young adults. Social Indicators Research, 95(3), 393–406.
Smith, P. K., Mahdavi, J., Carvalho, M., Fisher, S., Russell, S., & Tippett, N. (2008). Cyberbullying: Its nature and impact in secondary school pupils. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, 49(4), 376-385.
Sofka, C. J. (2009). Adolescents, technology, and the internet: Coping with loss in the digital world. Adolescent encounters with death, bereavement, and coping, 155-173.
Stearns, C. Z., & Stearns, P. N. (1989). Anger: The struggle for emotional control in America’s history. University of Chicago Press.
Stoneman, Z., & Brody, G. H. (1993). Sibling relations in the family context. Paul H. Brookes Publishing.
Sullivan, H. S. (1937). A note on the implications of psychiatry, the study of interpersonal relations, for investigations in the social sciences. American Journal of Sociology, 42(6), 848-861.
Sullivan, A., Parsons, S., Green, F., Wiggins, R. D., & Ploubidis, G. (2017). The path from social origins to top jobs: social reproduction via education. The British journal of sociology.
Swearer, S., & Hymel, S. (2015). Bullying and Discrimination in Schools: Exploring Variations Across Student Subgroups. School Psychology Review, 44(4), 504-509.
Tannen, D., Hamilton, H. E., & Schiffrin, D. (2015). The handbook of discourse analysis. John Wiley & Sons.
Thomas, L. A., De Bellis, M. D., Graham, R., & LaBar, K. S. (2007). Development of emotional facial recognition in late childhood and adolescence. Developmental science, 10(5), 547-558.
Thorne, B. (1993). Gender play: Girls and boys in school. New Brunswick: Rutgers University.
Tolman, D. L., & Tolman, D. L. (2009). Dilemmas of desire: Teenage girls talk about sexuality. Harvard University Press.
Troth, A., & Peterson, C. C. (2000). Factors predicting safe-sex talk and condom use in early sexual relationships. Health Communication, 12(2), 195-218.
Valkenburg, P. M., Sumter, S. R., & Peter, J. (2011). Gender differences in online and offline self‐disclosure in preadolescence and adolescence. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29(2), 253-269.
Walden, T. A., & Ogan, T. A. (1988). The development of social referencing. Child development, 1230-1240.
Warnick, A. L. (2015). Supporting youth grieving the dying or death of a sibling or parent: considerations for parents, professionals, and communities. Current opinion in supportive and palliative care, 9(1), 58-63.
Williams, A. L., & Merten, M. J. (2009). Adolescents’ online social networking following the death of a peer. Journal of Adolescent Research, 24(1), 67-90.
Wilson, M. N. (1986). The Black extended family: An analytical consideration. Developmental Psychology, 22(2), 246.
Wingate, S. (2017). Developing through bereavement: Dealing with loss during adolescence: Dealing with Dying, Death, and Grief During Adolescence by David E. Balk (Contributors: Corinne Cavuoti and Anne M. Smith). New York, NY: Routledge, 2014. 265 pp.(ISBN: 978-0-415-53450-50). $39.95 (paperback).
Young, J. T., Rebellon, C. J., Barnes, J. C., & Weerman, F. M. (2014). Unpacking the black box of peer similarity in deviance: Understanding the mechanisms linking personal behavior, peer behavior, and perceptions. Criminology, 52(1), 60-86.
Zimmerman, B. J. (2000). Attaining self-regulation: A social cognitive perspective. In Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 13-39).